designer bags cyber monday

designer bags cyber monday>designer bags cyber monday
camera bag cyber monday

Try playing on Hard mode and prove you're the X O game champion of the world! XO game is a fun, traditional, and online browser game where you have to use your own strategies of placing 3 marks in a horizontal, vertical, or diagonal row. The Gameplay of XO Tic Tac Toe Game: XO Tic Tac Toe brings you the most exciting gameplay. , 5x5 size board or 3x3 size board. You will have the option to choose one size board and you will play this game against our AI opponent. That player will win the game who will mark align three signs in any direction i. 7. It is a game played in landscape and it's playable on Desktop and Mobile on gamepix. This game has been published on 2015-12-01, and updated on 2020-02-18.

camera bag cyber monday

Fake news is pernicious as it spreads misleading and false information. Lazer et al.1 distinguish between misinformation (false or misleading information) and disinformation (false information that is purposely spread to deceive people). In both information contexts, fake news is likely to promote misperceptions. Recent studies show that the information overload during the COVID-19 pandemic increased the likelihood of fake news sharing by increasing consumers' psychological strain2,3. By altering people's perceptions and attitudes, fake news can shape public debates about critical policy issues such as vaccinations or immigration. For instance, Allcott and Gentzkow4 suggest that in the 2016 presidential election in the United States, fake news had a considerable impact and could even have been crucial in determining the election's outcome. Brady et al.5 contend that moral-emotional language in political messages substantially increases their diffusion within (and less so between) ideological group boundaries. Our second contribution is that we simultaneously study detecting as well as accidental and deliberate sharing of fake news in an experimental setting. Deliberate sharing means that respondents know that the news they are willing to share is unlikely to be true; accidental sharing occurs if respondents share fake news, which they erroneously believe to be true. To distinguish between accidental and deliberate sharing, we must elicit whether respondents can correctly identify fake news items. Several papers focused on either the detection19,20,21,22,23 or on the sharing of fake news3,11,12,15,17,24, but not both. Further, Buchanan14 distinguishes between accidental and deliberate sharing of fake news but does so with a retrospective survey question only. Accidental and deliberate sharing is not measured through actual sharing behavior or through the intention to share an actual news item. Instead, respondents are asked in retrospect whether they have ever shared fake news accidentally or deliberately. We gave the respondents ten viral news headlines from the internet, five being "fake news" and the other five being true news stories. We selected headlines that avoided any national bias for the UK or Germany. None of the headlines deal with domestic affairs in Germany or the UK as this would facilitate the validation for respondents from the respective country. We also avoided headlines associated with national policy stances to avoid any partisan bias. We pretested the headlines among Ph.D. students to ensure sufficient variation in response. Table 2 presents pairwise correlations between the variables, while Table 3 displays regression results for the individual-level determinants of two fake news indicators: (a) how good respondents are in separating fake news from accurate ones (Column 1 for Germany and Column 2 for the United Kingdom); and (b) how likely they are to spread fake news stories (Columns 3 and 4). We will focus on the regression results because we can measure the relationship between variables net of the other variables and we can also make the interpretation more explicit. The outcome in the first two columns is a scale constructed from the number of headlines correctly identified by a respondent. A higher value indicates better detection (see "Methods" Section). In the first two columns, we explain the respondents' ability to identify fake news headlines with various individual-level factors including the respondent's political orientation. The outcome is replaced in the last two columns by a scale constructed from the number of fake statements a respondent is willing to spread. Hence, we explain the willingness to spread misinformation (deliberately or accidentally) with individual-level variables. We can also investigate the country differences in more detail. Supplement Table 2 repeats the regressions for detecting and sharing fake news, but here the data for the two countries are pooled, and a country dummy is added. In line with our hypothesis, UK respondents detect fake news less accurately and share it more frequently. Supplement Fig. 1 in the Online Appendix illustrates the differences between the two countries regarding the ability to detect fake news in a box plot. We have also tested whether there are any statistically significant differences between the coefficients of individual-level determinants in the two countries for the regressions without the country dummy presented in Table 3. For fake news detection, the Wald test of equality of coefficients suggests that only gender coefficients are significantly different between the two countries at the 5% level (p = 0.009). Thus, although women detect fake news less frequently than men in both countries, the difference between women and men in detecting fake news is significantly larger in Germany than in the UK. For fake news sharing, gender (p = 0.020), age (0.000), and both political orientation coefficients (p = 0.050 and p = 0.006) are significantly different between the two countries. Figure 1 displays the share of respondents who would like to share at least one fake-news statement. The percentages are presented separately for deliberate and accidental sharing and for the two countries. There is more accidental than deliberate sharing. Around 12 percent of respondents want to share at least one news item that they have correctly identified as fake news. In Supplement Table 1, we observe that these respondents are disproportionately from the UK, male, and right on the political spectrum compared to the respondents who did not share any fake news. More than 19 percent (17 percent in Germany, 21 percent in the UK) want to share accidentally, i.e., at least one of the news items they want to share is not correctly identified as fake news. A striking difference emerges in the percentage of respondents who accidentally share a large number of fake news headlines. In Germany, only 6 percent accidentally share three or more fake news headlines, whereas the percentage is 13 percent in the United Kingdom. Our results, which highlight the importance of individual characteristics in understanding recently elevated misinformation campaigns, have similarities and differences with previous findings in the literature. Guess, Nagler, and Tucker13 and Grinberg et al.11 find-similar to our analysis-that it is primarily the more conservative individuals who share fake news. Both articles argue that older cohorts share much more fake news than the younger ones, whereas the opposite is true in our study and in Buchanan's study14. As in the case of Germany, Preston et al.10 identify higher abilities for fake news detection among those with better education. We find that those on the right on the political spectrum share fake news more often than those on the left. Similarly, Allcott and Gentzkow4 argued that, in the 2016 US election, fake news was widely shared in favor of Donald Trump. By contrast, Hopp, Ferrucci, and Vargo12 found partial support (on Facebook, but not on Twitter) to connect ideological extremity (left and right) to fake news sharing. The headlines were presented in text-form and not combined with any visual material, which might suggest the appearance in specific (social) media outlets. We then asked the respondents "To the best of your knowledge, how likely is it that the claim in each of the headlines is correct?" with five answer possibilities: (a) extremely unlikely, (b) somewhat unlikely, (c) neither likely nor unlikely, (d) somewhat likely, and (e) extremely likely. We operationalized fake news detection as a scale ranging from 0 to 40 with a higher value indicating better fake news detection. For fake news headlines, we gave a value of 4 if the respondent answered 'extremely unlikely', 3 for 'somewhat unlikely', 2 for 'neither likely nor unlikely', 1 for 'somewhat likely', and 0 for 'extremely likely'. The reversed coding was used for true headlines. (2) What Is Arbitrage in Sports Betting? The bettor can place a bet on both outcomes, ensuring a profit regardless of which team wins. 5 wins, you profit $120 on Caesars and lose $117. 65 on PointsBet, profiting $2. No matter what happens, you turn a profit. You should be warned, some sportsbooks may limit how much you are allowed to bet or even ban accounts that are identified as using an arbitrage betting strategy. Live betting odds can also provide arbitrage opportunities, as odds can change during the course of a sporting event. Arbitrage betting allows bettors to lock in a profit regardless of the outcome of a sporting event, like the Super Bowl. Get the BetMGM Bonus Code2. A substantial amount of match markets are available days before kick off and futures markets like National Championship odds, Heisman Trophy odds, Conference and even Divisional winners are easily accessible and live for the majority of each gameweek. Live Betting College Football Same Game Parlay? College Football Promos (/5) College Football Market Depth (/5) App Available? ✓ ✓ 4/5 5/5 ✓ Given Borgata uses the same sportsbook as BetMGM, you'll find similar College Football markets that you would there. Their game lines are released early, with fantastic parlay options and futures markets. The biggest contests and occasions will attract the best offers, such as opening weekend, or most obviously, the national championship. Bonus bets are one of the sign-up offers you're likely to come across the most, with College Football betting sites often using them to try and convince people into using their services to bet with. How do I bet on the College Football online? cheap burberry bags wholesale